What biases should forensic psychologists be aware of as an expert what is the most important source of bias?

Forensic psychologists and psychiatrists are ethically bound to be impartial when performing evaluations or providing expert opinions in court. But new research suggests that courtroom experts’ evaluations may be influenced by whether their paycheck comes from the defense or the prosecution. The research is published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

The findings reveal that experts who believed they were working for prosecutors tended to rate sexually violent offenders as being at greater risk of re-offending than did experts who thought they were working for the defense.

“We were surprised by how easy it was to find this ‘allegiance effect,’” says psychological scientist Daniel Murrie of the University of Virginia. “The justice system relies often on expert witnesses, and most expert witnesses believe they perform their job objectively — these findings suggest this may not be the case.”

Murrie and co-author Marcus Boccaccini at Sam Houston State have worked in forensic psychology for years, watching the adversarial justice system use forensic experts to gain an advantage in their cases.

“We became increasingly curious about whether forensic psychologists and psychiatrists could actually do what their ethical codes prescribed: handling each case objectively, regardless of what side retained them,” says Murrie.

Murrie and Boccaccini decided to conduct a “real world” experiment, providing 118 experienced forensic psychiatrists and psychologists from several states the opportunity to participate in a two-day workshop covering the psychological tests used to evaluate sexually violent predators. In exchange, the experts agreed to provide paid consultation to a state agency that was supposedly reviewing a large batch of sexually violent offender case files.

The experts returned weeks later to meet with an actual attorney and score risk assessment instruments for offenders as part of the paid consultation – unbeknownst to them, each expert was given the same four files to review.

Even though the experts used the same well-known assessment instruments to evaluate the same offenders, the risk scores they assigned turned out to be significantly different depending on who they thought was paying them: Those who believed they were hired by the prosecution tended to assign higher risk scores, while those who believed they were hired by the defense assigned lower risk scores.

Murrie notes that most people in this line of work really do try to be objective, and not every expert in the study demonstrated biased scoring. But the findings suggest that some of the experts were swayed by the side that retained them.

“In short, even experts were vulnerable to the same biases as the rest of us, in ways that left them less objective than they thought,” says Murrie.

The researchers hope that the study will prompt experts in his field to take a hard look at how evaluators are trained and how they practice.

“Demonstrating that allegiance is a problem is the first step towards solving the problem,” Murrie concludes. “The justice system certainly needs the expertise experts can offer, but the system also needs to be able to trust that their input is truly objective.”

The abstract for this article can be found online.

Co-authors on this research include Lucy Guarnera from the University of Virginia and Katrina Rufino from Sam Houston State University.

This research was supported by the National Science Foundation Law & Social Science Program.

Forensic psychologists and psychiatrists are ethically bound to be impartial when performing evaluations or providing expert opinions in court. But new research suggests that courtroom experts' evaluations may be influenced by whether their paycheck comes from the defense or the prosecution. The research is published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

The findings reveal that experts who believed they were working for prosecutors tended to rate sexually violent offenders as being at greater risk of re-offending than did experts who thought they were working for the defense.

"We were surprised by how easy it was to find this 'allegiance effect,'" says psychological scientist Daniel Murrie of the University of Virginia. "The justice system relies often on expert witnesses, and most expert witnesses believe they perform their job objectively -- these findings suggest this may not be the case."

Murrie and co-author Marcus Boccaccini at Sam Houston State have worked in forensic psychology for years, watching the adversarial justice system use forensic experts to gain an advantage in their cases.

"We became increasingly curious about whether forensic psychologists and psychiatrists could actually do what their ethical codes prescribed: handling each case objectively, regardless of what side retained them," says Murrie.

Murrie and Boccaccini decided to conduct a "real world" experiment, providing 118 experienced forensic psychiatrists and psychologists from several states the opportunity to participate in a two-day workshop covering the psychological tests used to evaluate sexually violent predators. In exchange, the experts agreed to provide paid consultation to a state agency that was supposedly reviewing a large batch of sexually violent offender case files.

The experts returned weeks later to meet with an actual attorney and score risk assessment instruments for offenders as part of the paid consultation -- unbeknownst to them, each expert was given the same four files to review.

Even though the experts used the same risk well-known assessment instruments to evaluate the same offenders, the risk scores they assigned turned out to be significantly different depending on who they thought was paying them: Those who believed they were hired by the prosecution tended to assign higher risk scores, while those who believed they were hired by the defense assigned lower risk scores.

Murrie notes that most people in this line of work really do try to be objective, and not every expert in the study demonstrated biased scoring. But the findings suggest that some of the experts were swayed by the side that retained them.

"In short, even experts were vulnerable to the same biases as the rest of us, in ways that left them less objective than they thought," says Murrie.

The researchers hope that the study will prompt experts in his field to take a hard look at how evaluators are trained and how they practice.

"Demonstrating that allegiance is a problem is the first step towards solving the problem," Murrie concludes. "The justice system certainly needs the expertise experts can offer, but the system also needs to be able to trust that their input is truly objective."

make a difference: sponsored opportunity

Story Source:

Materials provided by Association for Psychological Science. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.

Journal Reference:

  1. D. C. Murrie, M. T. Boccaccini, L. A. Guarnera, K. A. Rufino. Are Forensic Experts Biased by the Side That Retained Them? Psychological Science, 2013; DOI: 10.1177/0956797613481812


Voir dire of experts as to credentials and experience needs to be viewed as only one aspect of the vetting process of forensic work product. Experts, as much as anyone else, are prone to conscious and unconscious sources of bias. This rather significant problem area in forensic reports is discussed by (Stuld and Simon, 2013) under the rubric of heuristic and cognitive biases. Heuristics addresses how people arrive at decisions or reach conclusions in complex family law, civil, or criminal matters. Errors in this area are of concern when experts focus or overly attend to only a part of the problem or afford overemphasis to certain factors and minimize the contribution of other factors. One can also see this problem in medicine. A patient may complain of pain in a particular body region whereas the provider focuses only another medical issue.

The sources of cognitive biases are unfamiliarity with Best Practice Standards, ignorance of the research literature and use of methods not supported by the literature, eg, play therapy, anatomical dolls in assessment of child sexual abuse allegations.

Anchoring Bias

This heuristic is characterized by focusing on only selected information during the evaluation at the expense of other information. This results in not being able to see the forest through the trees. Once an expert is settled on a particular "set" of assumptions, all other assumptions are interpreted consistent with the initial assumptions. This writer has heard a litany of complaints from parents concerning child custody evaluators being biased against them in reports. This particular bias is also seen in the police investigation wherein investigators have already arrived at conclusions about a defendant before the investigative interview concludes. This then leads to comments to investigators who "contaminate" the interview. One also comes across rebuttal experts who "always" seem to opine on the "side" of retaining counsel. Thus, individuals engage in "doctor shopping" to find these individuals.

Confirmatory Bias

A confirmatory bias is a tendency to form an opinion or impression prior to collecting all data and not considering other data at odds with the expert's initial opinion and impressions.

Stereotyping

This type of bias is seen when an evaluator is affected by characteristics of the individual being evaluated to the detriment of focus on all of the data. One frequently observes the effects of this type of bias on an evaluator's perception of a particular parent in a child custody evaluation. Data unfavorable to the evaluator's position with respect to the individual being evaluated is not given due consideration. Standards for experts for the Courts dictate objectivity by an expert with opinions grounded in data that are research driven not driven by the evaluator's biases or preconceptions. One sees this type of bias in cases where the evaluator disregards data from other third party sources or, in the case of Jodi Arias, the defense expert apparently did not interview anyone other than the defendant. This procedure is suspect as it depends solely on self-report with a defendant invested in presenting positively skewed information in order to extricate themselves from untenable legal situations.

Data Gathering Biases

A source of bias, viz, data gathering bias, results in evaluators placing more credence on certain types of data and procedures than others, viz, play therapy as opposed to objective psychological testing in assessment of child sexual abuse allegations. This writer has seen evaluators draw conclusions on parenting and bonding in child custody evaluations solely on the basis of a scale score on the MMPI-2-RF.

Research Bias

This writer has seen experts who cite only research to support their position while giving no attention to research not supporting their position. The research they present is cherry-picked to support their opinion and point of view.

Third Party Observers (TPOs)

Some evaluators have third parties in the examination room during forensic assessments. The literature speaks in unison on this point, viz, forensic assessments are not standardized with respect to other individuals being in the examining room with the evaluator and subject. The result of this is a non-standardized forensic assessment, resulting in questionable reliability. This arrangement distorts the examiner-subject field. A third-party, especially a party who has antipathy towards another party in the room with the child being interviewed, has a significant adverse influence on the response set of the subject being interviewed.

Additional Factors Constituting Forensic Frye and Daubert Evidentiary Standards

It is important to vet an expert not just according to credentials and experience, but also conduct a critique of their methodology. Whereas the Frye "General Acceptance" Standard requires teaching based upon methodology generally accepted in the scientific community, Daubert requires the theory and/or techniques utilized by the expert to have been tested (falsifiability), whether techniques utilized have been suggested to peer review, error rate(s) of the technique and criteria governing application of techniques.

Concluding Comments

Attorneys and consumers of forensic psychological assessments would be wise to view forensic assessments through the lens of guidelines (Forensic Psychology Speciality Guidelines, American Psychological Association Practice Guidelines for Child Custody Evaluations) as well as the other criteria discussed in this paper. Clients and the Courts deserve no less!

Dr. Perrotti received his PhD in Clinical Psychology from Alliant University in San Diego, CA. He is a licensed psychologist in California and Pennsylvania. Dr. Perrotti is a member of the National Register of Health Service Provider in psychology and the National Academy of Neuropsychology. He was an Assistant Professor of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences at the Keck School of Medicine, USC from 2005-2006. Dr. Perrotti is the author of numerous publications in forensic psychology and assessment, traumatic brain injury in college, professional sports and military populations, and child trauma and complex PTSD.

©Copyright - All Rights Reserved

DO NOT REPRODUCE WITHOUT WRITTEN PERMISSION BY AUTHOR.

2/18/2005· Sexual Abuse - Molestation - Harassment

When Adults Become Abusers of Young People

By: Rev. David O'Leary, STL, Dphil

The statements are always the same: "We did not see any warning signs." Accounts of child sexual abuse shock our senses. But there are some warning signs people should be aware of. Although they should not lead to witch hunts against innocent people, they should raise people's level of alertness

10/2/2018· Sexual Abuse - Molestation - Harassment

The Influence of Psychotic Illness on Victim Credibility in Allegations of Child Sexual Abuse

By: Dr. Mark McDonald

In the absence of physical evidence or eyewitness testimony, establishing a criminal charge of child sexual abuse often boils down to a “he said, she said” weighing of credibility of the accuser and accused. As the burden of proof lies on the prosecution to establish the charge beyond a reasonable doubt, witness testimony becomes paramount in determining the outcome of the case. If the alleged victim is suffering from a serious mental illness, such as psychosis, that impairs one’s ability to either accurately recall the alleged abuse or distinguish reality from fantasy, witness credibility will suffer, and the defendant will likely be found not guilty. For both plaintiff and defense counsel, when there is any suspicion of psychotic illness in the accuser, it is critical to assess the following before trial: type and severity of mental illness, genetics, drug use, previous abuse, and neglect.

9/10/2014· Sexual Abuse - Molestation - Harassment

Campus Sexual Assault and Harassment Lawsuits: Title IX Standards and Questions of Liability

By: Dr. Edward Dragan

In 2011, the U.S. Department of Education Office for Civil Rights (OCR) issued a "Dear Colleague" letter to college and university administrators about implementation of Title IX of the Education Amendments of 1972 in regards to campus sexual assault cases. Title IX prohibits discrimination on the basis of sex in education programs or activities in schools that receive federal funding. The letter explains that schools are required to develop and distribute policies regarding sexual harassment, designate a Title IX coordinator to oversee the school's duties, train staff and students in sexual harassment and violence issues, and establish an investigation procedure and an adjudication process. The letter did not articulate specific procedural safeguards, rules for the examination of evidence, or guidelines for the conduct of adjudication or hearing processes for cases of campus sexual violence.

Última postagem

Tag